The Relationship Between Locus of Control, Sport Competition Anxiety,
and Superstitious Behavior
Emily Mouyard
Abstract
The relationship between locus of
control, sport competition anxiety, and superstition was examined among five
collegiate athletic teams. Participants included both men and women
undergraduate collegiate athletes at a midwestern
National Collegiate Athletic Association (NCAA) Division II university.
Participants completed a survey
packet that was three fold. The purpose of the packet was to gather information
on athletes’ superstitious beliefs and behavior; to assess individuals’ level of
locus of control; and to evaluate the level of sport competition anxiety
experienced by each participant. It was predicted that athletes engage in
superstitious behavior as a result of their locus of control, gender, and level
of experienced sport competition anxiety. It was concluded that there is a
relationship between athletes with an external locus of control and
superstition. Also, results indicated a gender effect on superstitious beliefs
and behaviors. In addition, the notion that sport competition anxiety and
superstition was correlated was not supported.
Keywords:
locus of control, illusory
correlation, superstition
Superstitions in Collegiate Athletes
It is a common occurrence in the sports world for an athlete to engage in
superstitious behaviors that may seem odd to others. Society seems to mock
athletes and believe that their behaviors are absurd, however, each superstition
serves a purpose in which the sportsperson finds to be beneficial to their
performance. These behaviors are present in athletes of all ages, especially
professionals. For instance, St. Louis Cardinals’ second baseman, Skip Schumaker,
continuously readjusts his batting gloves before and during his at bat
(Cardinals Diamond Diaries, 2010). Another example would be St. Louis Cardinals’
2011 World Series MVP David Freese, hanging a batman mask in his locker during
each game to ensure success for both the team and him. In addition, Cardinals’
catcher, Yadier Molina, wears the same necklace every game and kisses the mitt
pendent after every pitch during his at bat (Cardinals Diamond Diaries, 2010).
These behaviors may not seem too far out
of the norm when it comes to athletes; however, there are some who take their
superstitious beliefs and behaviors to the extreme. NBA sensation, Michael
Jordan, would wear his college uniform shorts underneath his Chicago bulls
uniform (Cox, 2010). In fact, it is said that he is the reason for the trend of
long shorts in the sport of basketball because he would wear extra-large uniform
bottoms to hide his University of North Carolina shorts. Caron Butler, forward
for the Los Angeles Clippers, feels that he has to chew a minimum of twelve
straws throughout a competition (Cox, 2010). Legendary Chicago Cubs pitcher Turk
Wendell would brush his teeth in the dugout between each inning. Lastly,
all-star tennis player Serena Williams will bring her shower sandals to every
match and place them next to her bag for good luck (Cox, 2010).
So why do athletes engage in these behaviors? What purpose do they serve
and do athletes really believe that these actions will affect their performance?
According
to Dr. Richard Lustberg, creator of the Web site
Psychology of Sports,
superstitious behavior and rituals play a significant role in the mental
preparation for a competition (Chick, 2008). Superstitions
in a sport can be defined as actions that are repetitive, formal, sequential,
and distinct from technical performance that the athlete believes to be powerful
in controlling luck or external factors (Schippers & Van Lange, 2004).
Superstitions are grouped into three categories; they are rituals, taboos, and
fetishes (Weir, 2003). For instance, rituals are routines that must be done
before every competition in order to ensure success. Taboos are identified as
actions that must be done in order to avoid bad luck. In addition, fetishes are
considered as lucky charms, which give athletes a material object that help them
perform well. Despite what people may believe, success is determined by the
skill of the athlete and the opponent, not the superstition (Weir, 2003).
There is a fine line between a comforting routine and a superstition. The primary reason that athletes
engage in rituals is to provide themselves with a level of comfort and attempt
to have a sense of control within a situation (Cox, 2010). When it comes to the
performance of the opposing team, the outcome is completely uncontrollable;
however, having a set ritual helps athletes feel that they are in the right
mental state to perform (Cox, 2010).
Psychologists commonly define the superstitious response as an authority through
which an answer is maintained from an accidental correlation with reinforcement
(Brevers, Bernard, Xavier, & Frederic, 2011).
“A causal superstition is suggested to be part
of a conscious belief; while a coincidental superstition may be more ambiguous
about what behaviors individuals believed caused a particular outcome” (Wright &
Erdal, 2008, p.188).
The occurrence of superstitions can be explained in
terms of an illusion of control. This tendency is described as individuals
believing they can control events or influence outcomes that they are physically
incapable of having an impact on. Another explanation for athletes engaging in
superstitious behavior is based on the Lemer’s Just World Hypothesis that
suggests that people need to believe in the equity and stability of their
environment (Brevers et al. 2011).
“This need may be relevant in dealing with the psychological tension generated
by the uncertainty and importance of competitive situations and mediated by
levels of competition and personal involvement (athletic identity)”
(Brevers et al. 2011, p.5).
Before one can understand the causation of superstitious beliefs,
behaviors and rituals, there needs to be a clear distinction between concepts.
Far more athletes will admit to having a set of rituals than superstitions
(Durham, 2010). Rituals are frequently classified as a pre-game mental
preparation; rituals consist of putting on apparel in a particular order,
listening to certain songs, or doing the same warm-ups routine. Superstitions on
the other hand, have been found to be more psychologically depth. Superstitions
are typically related to a particular instance of where an athlete has equated
something they have done previously that has brought them success
(Durham, 2010). Athletes’ superstitions are not as simple as their rituals.
Sport psychologists indicate that an athlete will claim having a superstition
because it gives them a mental edge over their opponent. In comparison, other
psychologists point to the increased anxiety or lack of confidence caused if an
athlete were to abandon the superstition practices or routines (Durham, 2010).
Many may feel that an athlete’s rituals and superstitions are nonsense;
however, facts have shown that these routines may be beneficial to the player.
Researchers have found that superstitions and rituals give athletes a sense of
control, with that; they believe that these behaviors will grant them success
(Chick, 2008). Superstitions and rituals have also been found to significantly
regulate psychological tension that results from an athlete’s level of
competition anxiety (Damisch, Stoberock, & Mussweiler, 2010).
Athletes use these strategies
unconsciously to mentally prepare for a competition when tension and anxiety are
high. Even though many athletes may have different reasoning, the majority of
superstitions are developed because the player has related two random events
together, therefore generating an illusory correlation (Durham, 2010).
A study was conducted to examine how superstitious behavior varied among
different athletic teams and what motivated participants to engage in these acts
(Bleak & Frederick, 1998). The study provided information about the use of
superstitions and perceived effectiveness among three
National Collegiate Athletic Association (NCAA)
Division I teams. Researchers predicted a positive relationship between use of
superstition rituals, level of anxiety and perceived importance of success.
Researchers believed that as more importance is placed on the outcome of the
competition, sport anxiety levels will increase and the athlete will engage in
superstitious behavior to relieve tension and attempt control the end result
(Bleak & Frederick, 1998). Results did not support the prediction that
religiosity, locus of control, importance of success, and anxiety contribute to
superstitious behavior and rituals (Bleak & Frederick, 1998). Another study was
conducted to examine the
interactions between perceived controllability, optimism,
pessimism, and superstition (Rudski, 2004). Researchers predicted that the
illusion of control, superstition, and optimism would also be positively
related. Researchers predicted that when tasks are viewed as uncontrollable,
people engage in superstitious behavior and generate an illusion of control.
Data supported the idea that random and uncontrollable circumstances are also
the antecedent conditions for the display of illusion of control. In addition,
illusion of control is presented as a demonstration of unwarranted optimism in
controlling the uncontrollable (Rudski, 2004). These studies have supplied
evidence that supports the notion that athletes engage in superstitious behavior
in an attempt to have a heightened sense of comfort and influence the outcome.
Athletes tend to associate particular behaviors with unrelated events, such as
an elite performance, therefore causing a need to continuously repeat actions (Rudski,
2004).
In a series of experiments,
Langer (1975) found that people irrationally overestimate their likelihood of
success of a situation by having a sense of control (Rudski & Edwards, 2007).
The illusory sense of control becomes more likely when competition anxiety and
importance are high. The illusion of control or illusory correlation is present
when an individual finds a cause and effect relationship between two or more
unrelated events.
Regardless of how the illusion of control is measured, all dependent
measures reflect a belief that one’s actions or rituals can influence an outcome
that is, in fact, outside their control
(Rudski
& Edwards, 2007). Within the literature, it appears as though Langer indicated
that athletes put emphasis on their sense of illusion of control; an illusion of
control exists when an individual develops an illusory correlation and then
begins to believe that they can perform a particular act and control a random
event. An athlete will develop the notion that they have the ability to control
the outcome of the competition by repetitively doing a specific act or even by
having object in their possession (Rudski & Edwards, 2007). Since many
psychologists see this heuristic as false, the actions are often mocked as
maladaptive. Nonetheless, data shows that an illusion of control can give even
the most non-superstitious person a sense of control and comfort (Rudski &
Edwards, 2007).
Another notion
that contributes to superstitious beliefs and behaviors is the locus of control.
Locus of control is defined
as an expectancy of a personal success probability that
exceeds the objective probability of the outcome (Hageman, 2006). In other
words, it is the amount of perceived control that an individual feels they have
of their life. This mind set is caused by an event that occurs by chance but is
associated with factors that are a result of skill-based situations (Hageman,
2006).
In one study, researchers
predicted that people with
a perceived external locus of control are
more likely to feel comfortable in a situation that is viewed as uncontrollable
(Tennen & Sharp, 1983). Those with an external locus of control are also doubly
impaired in that they tend to view outcomes as response independent and still
succumb to the illusion of control. Previous data supports the hypothesis that
those with internal locus of control will expand effort and those with an
external locus of control are more likely to give up due to the veridical
perception of response (Tennen & Sharp, 1983). The question still remains as to
who is more likely to develop the sense of illusion of control and engage in
superstitious behavior? A study was conducted to examine
the practice of superstitious behavior and how it is
related to type A behavior pattern, athletic identity, and locus of control
among NCAA teams (Todd & Brown, 2003).
“B. F. Skinner (1948) discussed the
acquisition of superstition as a conditioning process that occurs as a function
of temporal relation only; the mere fact that reinforcement coincides temporally
with a response does not mean that it is contingent upon the response”
(Todd & Brown, 2003, p.169).
As a result of the fact that skill, talent, and physical
fitness are characteristics that the majority of collegiate athletes share, they
may feel pressure and uncertainty about the competition. Research found that
athletes with more experience tend to feel higher levels of anxiety about
competition importance therefore causing them to engage in superstitious
behavior to gain an upper hand on their opponent (Todd & Brown, 2003).
These behaviors are often times viewed as unnecessary, but do they work?
Does the commitment that is made to superstitious behaviors and rituals
beneficial or is it harmful to the individual psychologically?
The question of whether superstitions are a sign of
obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD) has been brought up due to the fact that
people who suffer from this condition often have to repetitively perform
particular rituals every day (Albert, 2012). Despite many opinions, evidence
shows that even though OCD may mimic superstitious behavior, there is not a
connection between the two. Superstitions exist because of our need for more
control in a world that is in fact uncontrollable. Studies have shown that
superstitions can have both a positive and negative psychological effect on an
individual (Albert, 2012). People often misperceive random events as being
related when in reality they are not. Individuals also believe that they can
control random uncontrollable events. These cognitive heuristics are so powerful
that people allow them to have dominance over certain aspects of their life,
which can be psychologically damaging and detrimental to one’s confidence
(Albert, 2012). A study was conducted on frequent hockey gamblers to examine the
affect that perceived control could have on an individual’s performance, even at
guessing outcomes. It was hypothesized that the accuracy of predictions made by
participants would be superior to the accuracy of randomly selected wagers
(Cantinotti, Ladouceur, & Jacques, 2004). Researchers
found that the gamblers’ predictions were more accurate when they were more
confident in their control of the outcome (Cantinotti et al. 2004).
Even though the
notion of athletes engaging in this behavior may sound unconventional, evidence
suggests that it is effective. It is evident that routines and rituals offer
athletes a sense of familiarity and comfort, which in turn provides the
individual with a feeling of control and confidence. Nonetheless, rituals
serve one very significant purpose; they mentally prepare an athlete for the
game and promote focus during competition (Fogelman, 2012). Sports psychologists
suggest that if the athlete believes that their routines and rituals work, then
they will (Fogelman, 2012). Superstitions have been shown to lower anxiety,
build confidence, and reduce the feeling of uncertainty (Fogelman, 2012). The
belief that the actual behavior or ritual itself affects the outcome is false;
however it has significant meaning to the athlete and their mental preparation
for a competition or performance. Even though superstitions are inconsequential
creations of the mind, putting in the extra effort by execution can be
beneficial to one’s performance
(Damisch et al. 2010).
A study conducted showed that participants that engaged in their personal
superstitious behaviors and rituals performed better in various motor and
cognitive tasks compared to those who abandoned their beliefs (Damisch et
al.2010). In addition, a perceived level of self-efficiency settled the
performance enhancing effects. When an individual activated their superstition,
their belief that they can master a task improved their performance (Damisch et
al. 2010). In conclusion, this study tested and supported the prediction that
superstitions have a positive effect on performance when activated as a result
of an increase in confidence (Damisch et al. 2010).
Many sport psychologists view superstitions as nothing more than
reactions that begin with conditioning and boosting a placebo effect (Roenigk,
2010).
Some believe that superstitions and rituals are an attempt to manipulate fate
and act as a psychological placebo to athletes
(Robson, 2005). Many athletes cling
to superstitions to help navigate the numerous events that require high levels
of performance because a little psychological edge can be beneficial in
concentration and focus. The most
negative consequence that can occur from superstitions and rituals would be if
the athlete were to abandon them; doubt, anxiety, and worry might escalate and
performance may significantly suffer. In reality, if an athlete is unable to
follow their rituals or superstitions, their focus may be hindered (Fogelman,
2012).
It has been a concern for some that athletes putting emphasis on a ritual or
superstition can be detrimental to an athlete’s confidence. It was reported in
an article that in a situation where an athlete does not perform well, they may
lose faith in the superstition and not have anything to rely on to maintain a
sense of control
(Roenigk, 2010). Some professionals believe that athletes
should put more emphasis on their skills rather than assigning success to an
outside source even though the placebo effect does have a positive effect on
performance (Roenigk, 2010). When an athlete feels that there is a behavior or
object that is causing them to play well, they will actually perform better
because of the visualization and state of mind they have created (Roenigk,
2010).
When analyzing the effects superstitions and rituals have on an athlete’s
performance, it seems as though evidence suggests that they are beneficial.
Superstitions
are generally something developed in hindsight, unintentionally because an
athlete has linked two unrelated events together and as a result feel required
to repeat them for the same outcome (Quinn, 2010). Aside from the fact that
athletes believe that the behavior itself determines the outcome of the
performance or competition, the real value in superstitions and rituals is to
boost confidence and give an athlete a sense of control that the athlete is
receiving from them (Quinn, 2010). Superstitions give athletes confidence that
their rituals and routines are gaining them success.
Confidence is an
important aspect to athletes because if they feel confident, it is suggested
that they will perform well (Chick, 2008). Confidence is the key in athletic
performance; whether you are a player, coach or fan, you have to believe that
you will do well, in order to actually execute successfully. If an individual
thinks that they are able to control the situation, they will become overwhelmed
with the idea that they will do well no matter what (Chick, 2008).
The superstitions and rituals that athletes follow and believe in are very
similar to the strategies that sport psychologists use, even though the athletes
may not realize it.
Athletes believe that their superstitions enhance their
performance and alter the outcome of the competition, but in fact, practice and
confidence is the key to success in athletics (Mayberry, 2010). Sports
psychologists identify superstitions as a coping mechanism because they give
athletes a sense of control, which in turn lowers anxiety and increases
confidence. The positive side of superstitions is that an athlete’s confidence
will rise as long as they feel they are in control of the situation during
competition. Nevertheless, if the competition does not end as an athlete
prefers, then they may lose confidence in their superstition and not have the
confidence in themselves to continue to perform well (Mayberry, 2010). With all
the studies that have been conducted and data that has been collected, it is
still difficult to distinguish between superstitious behavior and mental
preparation, but in the end, they are both beneficial to an athlete (Schippers &
Van Lange, 2006).
Research Hypotheses
Previous
literature has discussed that superstitions are a result of one’s locus of
control, development of an illusory correlation, and the illusion of control
bias. Studies have also suggested that athletes use superstitions and rituals as
a method of lowering anxiety and containing a sense of control over a situation.
The current study will exam the relationship between locus of control, sport
competition anxiety, and superstitious beliefs and behaviors in collegiate
athletes. It is hypothesized that collegiate athletes with an internal locus of
control engage in superstitious behavior in attempt to control the outcome of
their performance and the competition. It is also predicted that athletes who
experience higher levels of sport competition anxiety are more likely to engage
in superstitious behavior. In addition, a third hypothesis was added to the
study that predicted that superstitious beliefs and behaviors are more prevalent
in collegiate women athletes than collegiate men athletes.
Method
Participants
The participants were undergraduate student athletes at a private
midwestern university that was under NCAA Division II standings. There were five
athletic teams chosen to be examined, they included
basketball, baseball, bowling, softball, and track and field. The teams were
chosen on the basis that they have both a men and women’s team. Participants
gave voluntary consent to engage in the study. Data were collected in accordance
with the ethical standards of the American Psychological Association (American
Psychological Association, 2010).
Materials
A three-fold survey packet was used to collect data from the members of
each athletic team. The survey packet included scales to measure sport
competition anxiety; levels of locus of control; and superstitious beliefs and
behaviors. Every athlete answered a set of standard demographic questions
designed for use in the study. Demographic information was collected about
participants’ sex and the sport in which the athlete was a member.
Sport
Competition Anxiety Test. This test measures the tendency of an athlete to
experience anxiety prior, during, and after a competition in a sport. The
primary use of this scale is to measure competitive trait anxiety. Test scoring
is based on 15 questions that ask the athlete how they feel prior, during,
after, and about a competition.
Each item is
answered on a three-point Likert scale (rarely, sometimes, often) and a summary
score ranging from less than 17 (low competitive trait anxiety) to more than 24
(high levels of anxiety) is computed for each respondent (Hypnosisworks, 2006).
Multidimensional Locus of Control.
To measure locus of control, a scale was adapted from the Levenson
Multidimensional Locus of Control Inventory (Levenson, 1981). The original
inventory measured three separate scales of
locus of control: Internal Scale, Powerful Others Scale, and Chance Scale
(Levenson, 1981). There were originally eight items on each scale which was
presented as one twenty-four item questionnaire (Levenson, 1981). The present
study modified the inventory and only measured two scales of locus of control:
Internal and Chance. The scale was presented to the participants as a sixteen
item inventory. The participants were asked to respond to each item using the
Likert scale, ranging one to five to indicate the extent in which they agreed or
disagreed with the statement.
Superstition Belief Measure. A
questionnaire was conducted for the present study to measure superstitious
beliefs and behaviors in athletes. The questionnaire consists of twelve items
that refer to superstitious beliefs, behaviors, and rituals. Participants were
asked to respond using a Likert Scale ranging one to five to indicate the extent
of which one agreed or disagreed with the statement.
Procedure
Athletes completed the survey packet as a team either before or after the
team’s practice. Each team was surveyed at separate times. Every member of the
teams was given an informed consent document before the survey packet was
distributed. Each athlete was required to read the statement and give written
consent that they understood the terms and were voluntarily participating. The
athlete then turned in the section with their signature and retained possession
of the statement. The athletes were then given and completed the three-fold
survey packet. Participants completed the Superstition Belief Measure,
Multidimensional Locus of Control Inventory (Levenson, 1981), and the Sport
Competition Anxiety Test (Hypnosisworks, 2006). Participants
read each statement carefully and used a Likert scale to answer items
accordingly.
Results
Data were entered for demographics, superstitious beliefs and behaviors;
levels of locus of control; and sport competition anxiety. There was a sample
total of 111 undergraduate student-athletes at a small midwestern NCAA Division
II university; 60.4 percent were men (67) and 39.6 percent were women (44).
There were five athletic teams that were studied; they were basketball,
baseball, bowling, softball, and track and field. Table 1 displays the cross
tabulation count and frequencies for each sport and gender.
When measuring superstitious behavior, higher scores represented higher levels
of superstition, while lower scores represented lower levels of superstition.
Questions one, six, nine, and ten were recoded so higher scores would then
represent higher levels of superstition. Items asked questions regarding
superstitious beliefs and behaviors in both athletics and in life situations.
Examples of questions asked included belief in zodiac signs; a broken mirror
means seven years of bad luck; effect of a jersey number; and belief in luck. A
total count of each item of superstition was computed for each participant and
then a total for superstition was computed.
To measure locus of control, an internal locus of control variable and
external chance locus of control variable was created. A total count for both
internal locus of control and external chance locus of control were computed
based on the questions that were answered for both. An additional variable,
named locus of control, was created to indicate the level of locus of control
for each participant. Depending on which level of locus of control had a higher
score, the participant was either labeled one for internal or two for external
chance.
Sports competition anxiety was measured with responses often, sometimes,
or never to a series of statements asking about tension and arousal before,
during, and after a competition. Higher scores represented higher levels of
sport competition anxiety, while lower scores represented lower levels of sport
competition anxiety. A total count of each item of sport competition anxiety was
computed for each participant.
Superstition and Locus of Control
In order to test the hypothesis that athletes with an internal locus of
control are more likely to engage in superstitious behavior than athletes with
an external chance locus of control, a Pearson biviariate correlation was
performed between superstition and internal locus of control. This analysis
indicated that there was not a significant relationship between internal locus
of control (M 27.82,
SD 4.85) and superstitious beliefs and
behaviors (M 31.67,
SD 5.89); R (df = 109) = .108, p=.257. However, this analysis
indicated a positive correlation between external chance locus of control (M
19.06, SD 4.85) and superstitious behavior (M 31.67, SD 5.89); R (df=109)=
.376, p < .000. This analysis suggests that athletes with an external chance
locus of control are more superstitious than athletes who have an internal locus
of control.
Sports Anxiety and Superstition
In order to test the hypothesis that athletes with higher levels
of sport anxiety are more likely to engage in superstitious behavior, a Pearson
Biviariate Correlation was performed. This analysis indicated that there was not
a significant relationship between sport anxiety (M
33.51, SD 5.23) and superstitious
behavior (M 31.67,
SD 5.89); R(df= 109) = .095, p = .319. Data suggests that there is
not a relationship between the level of sport competition anxiety that an
athlete experiences and their superstitious beliefs and behaviors.
Gender and Superstition
To test the hypothesis that women athletes are more superstitious then
men athletes, a one-way ANOVA was performed. Results indicated a significant
difference in superstitious behavior based on gender, F (1, 109) = 4.256, p
<.041. Data suggests that men athletes (M
32.59, SD 5.50) are more superstitious
than women athletes (M 30.27,
SD 6.24).
Additional Findings
A one-way ANOVA was performed to test which gender experienced higher
levels of sport competition anxiety. This analysis indicated that there was a
significant difference in sport competition anxiety based on gender, F(1, 109) =
4.856, p < .030. These results indicate that women athletes (M
34.84, SD 6.76) experience higher levels of sport competition anxiety than
men athletes (M 32.64,
SD 3.72).
A Pearson Biviariate Correlation was performed to analyze the
relationship between sport competition anxiety and locus of control. This
analysis indicated a significant relationship between sport competition anxiety
(M 33.51,
SD 5.23) and external chance locus of control (M 19.06, SD 4.85); R= (df
= 109) = .199, p < .036. Data suggests that athletes with an external/chance
locus of control experience higher levels of sport competition anxiety than
those who have an internal locus of control.
Discussion
There were three hypotheses to this study while examining
superstitious behavior in collegiate athletes. For instance, it was hypothesized
that collegiate athletes with an internal locus of control would be more likely
to engage in superstitious behavior than athletes with an external chance locus
of control in attempt to control the outcome of their performance and the
competition. A statistical analysis of the data showed no significant
relationship between internal locus of control and superstition; however, data
suggests that there was a significant relationship between external chance locus
of control and superstition. An individual’s locus of control can be a
contributing factor as to why superstitions develop, however, it is not a
determinant. A person with an internal locus of control has been found to
believe that they are in control of every aspect of their life and can master
their fate (Albert, 2012). In addition, those who have an external locus of
control believe that their lives are controlled by outside factors, such as the
power of others or luck (Albert, 2012). Studies have shown that people with an
internal locus of control will expend effort in order to ensure a preferred
outcome, while those with an external locus of control are more likely to give
up due to veridical perception of response
(Tennen & Sharp, 1983). With this
evidence, researchers also found that individuals with both levels of locus of
control engage in superstition as a result of an illusion of control (Tennen &
Sharp, 1983).
In addition, it was hypothesized that collegiate athletes that experience
higher levels of sport competition anxiety are more likely to engage in
superstitious behavior in order to cope with tension. A statistical analysis of
the data showed no significant relationship between sport competition anxiety
and superstition. Certified psychologist Dr. Lustberg remarked that athletes use
superstitions as a coping mechanism in order to deal with the pressure to
succeed (Mayberry, 2010). He found that when an athlete engages in their
superstition, confidence increases therefore causing an improvement in
performance (Mayberry, 2010). Researchers have examined the theory that
superstitious behavior is used as a coping mechanism for anxiety and stress
caused by competition importance. Researchers believed that as more importance
is placed on the outcome of the competition, sport anxiety levels will increase
and the athlete may engage in superstitious behavior to lower anxiety and
possibly control the end result (Bleak & Frederick, 1998). After collecting data
from completed survey packets, evidence of this study also did not support the
prediction of a positive relationship between sport competition anxiety and
superstition (Bleak & Frederick, 1998).
Also, it was hypothesized that superstitious beliefs and behaviors would
be more prevalent in women collegiate athletes than collegiate men athletes. A
statistical analysis of the data indicated a significant difference in
superstition based on gender; however, data suggests that superstitious behavior
is more prevalent in collegiate men athletes than in collegiate women athletes.
Gender differences and its affect on superstition is not a commonly studied
area, however, researchers have focused on the differences between sports in
both men and women. In one particular study researchers found, while examining
the affects of competition and opponent importance on superstition, that the
level of superstition does not vary among athletic teams (Schippers & Van Lange,
2006). This study also found that superstitious behavior is more prevalent in
high performing athletes than mediocre and lower level performing athletes (Schippers
& Van Lange, 2006).
One potential problem with the study may have been the type of questions
included in the superstition belief measure. The items asked questions referring
to superstitious beliefs and behavior in both athletics and everyday life
situations. Items that were not relevant to athletics may have caused a
discrepancy in the data needed to validate the study of superstitions in
collegiate athletes. Asking questions about superstitious beliefs and behavior
outside of sports did not provide beneficial data and may have been irrelevant
to the research.
Another potential problem may have been the setting in which data was
collected. Previously stated, data was gathered at each individual team’s
practice, either before or after, which could have an affect on participants’
focus and concentration to the survey packet. For instance, gathering data after
a practice when the athletes were tired may have influenced their responses to
the items on the surveys. If this factor did have an influence on the
participants’ responses, then there may be a discrepancy with the results.
Collecting data for every team at the same time may have prevented this issue;
however, the possibility of distractions would still be present. Collecting data
in an environment where athletes were around a larger population of their peers
and the opposite sex, may lead to a lack of concentration and perceived
importance in the survey.
A modified version of the present study may provide more of an insight on
the factors that cause superstitious beliefs and behaviors in collegiate
athletes. Revising items on the surveys may provide more support to the
research. For instance, adding demographic questions such as age, level of
education, and performance standing would provide information for more factors
that could be correlated with superstitious beliefs and behavior. Also, revising
the Superstitious Belief Measure by eliminating the items that do not refer to
athletics may further validate evidence. In addition to revising the survey
packet, collecting data in a setting that is more suitable for analytical
thinking could avoid any disparity in the results. Making these changes may
produce data that support past research on the causes of superstitious beliefs
and behavior in collegiate athletes.
If evidence from past research is valid, then superstitious beliefs and
behavior in collegiate athletes is a result of the individual’s locus of control
and sport competition anxiety. Performing more studies and exploring a variety
of variables would yield a greater insight to more possible causes and reasoning
behind superstition. Learning more about superstitions could assist sport
psychologists, coaches, and players in understanding how individual athletes
view the sport and effectively find strategies that can further enhance
performance.
References
Albert, S. (2012).
The Psychology of Superstition:
Is 'magical' thinking
hurting or helping
you? WebMD.
http://www.webmd.com/mental-health/features/psychology-of-superstition
Bleak, J.L. & Frederick, C.M.
(1998). Superstitious behavior in sport: Levels of effectiveness and
determinants of use in three college sports. Journal of Sport Behavior, Vol
21(1). Pp. 1-15.
Brevers, D., Bernard, D., Xavier, N., & Frederic, N.
(2011).
Sport Superstition: Mediation of Psychological Tension on
Non-Professional Sportsmen's Superstitious Rituals.Journal of Sport Behavior,
Vol. 34, issue 1, (3-24, 22).
Cantinotti, M., Ladouceur, R. & Jacques, C. (2004). Sports Betting: Can Gamblers
Beat
Randomness? Psychology of Addictive Behaviors.Vol 18(2).
Pp. 143-147. doi:
10.1037/0893-164X.18.2.143
Cardinal Diamond Diaries. (2010).
http://cardinaldiamonddiaries.blogspot.com/2010/05/superstitious.html
Chick, J. (2008).
Superstitions may not be so crazy after all.
Psychology of Sport.
http://psychologyofsports.com/2008/10/30/superstitions-may-not-be-so-crazy-after-all/
Cox, L. (2010).
Why Athletes Need Rituals and Superstitions. Abc news medical unit.
http://abcnews.go.com/Health/WellnessNews/experts-athletes-weird-rituals-chewing-straws-performance/story?id=9961246#.T076B5gqOL9
Damisch, L., Stoberock, B., &Mussweiler, T. (2010). Keep Your Fingers Crossed!
How
Superstition Improves Performance. Psychological Science
(Sage Publications Inc.), Vol. 21, Issue 7, (1014-1020, 7). doi:
10.1177/0956797610372631
Durham, J. (2010). How Athletes
Use Superstitions and Rituals. Motivation Expert.
http://www.motivationexpert.co.uk/how-athletes-use-superstitions-rituals.html
Fogelman, L. (2012). Do Rituals
Really Help Athletes. Expert Sports Performance.
http://expertsportsperformance.com/do-rituals-help/
Hageman, A. 2006. Illusion of
Control. York University.
http://www.istheory.yorku.ca/illusionofcontrol.htm
Hypnosisworks (2006).
http://hypnosisworks.org.uk/scat_test.pdf
Levenson, H. (1981). Differentiating among internality,
powerful others, and chance. In
H. Lefcourt (Ed.), Research with the Locus of
Control Construct. New York: Academic Press, pp. 15-63.
Mayberry, W. (2010).
Unearthing superstitions. Psychology of
Sports.
http://psychologyofsports.com/2010/06/08/unearthing-superstitions/
Quinn, E. (2010).
Why Do So Many Athletes, Have Superstitions and Rituals:
Do Superstitions Give Athletes a
Performance Edge? Sports Medicine.
http://sportsmedicine.about.com/od/sportspsychology/a/superstitions.htm
Robson, D. (2005). Players walk fine
line when it comes to superstitions. USA Today.
Roenigk, A. (2010). The Power of
Belief. Espn the magazine.
http://sports.espn.go.com/espn/news/story?id=5660039
Rudski, J. (2004). The illusion of
control, superstitious belief, and optimism. Current Psychology: A Journal for
Diverse Perspectives on Diverse Psychological Issues. Vol 22(4). Pp. 306-315.
doi:
10.1007/s12144-004-1036-8
Rudski, J.M. & Edwards, A.
(2007).
Malinowski Goes to
College: Factors Influencing
Students' Use of Ritual and Superstition. Journal of General Psychology, Vol.
134, Issue 4, (389-403, 15).
Schippers, M.C. & Van Lange, P.A.M. (2006). The Psychological Benefits of
Superstitious Rituals in Top Sport: A Study Among Top
Sportspersons. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, Vol 36(10), pp. 2532-2553.
doi:
10.1111/j.0021-9029.2006.00116.x
Tennen, H. & Sharp, J.P.. (1983).
Control orientation and the illusion of control. Journal
of
Personality Assessment, Vol 47(4). Pp. 369-374. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4704_6
Todd, M. & Brown, C. (2003).
Characteristics Associated with Superstitious Behavior in
Track and Field Athletes: Are There NCAA Divisional Level Differences? Journal
of Sport Behavior, issue 2, Vol. 26, (168, 20).
Weir, S.(2003). Supersitition--Can
it make you a winner? Challenge Newsline, issue 29, (8, 3/4).
Wright, P.B. & Erdal, K.J. (2008).
Sport Superstition as a Function of Skill Level and Task Difficulty. Journal of
Sport Behavior, issue 2, Vol. 31, (197-199, 13).
Table 1
Cross tabulation and Frequencies of Athletic Teams
and Gender
|
Frequency |
Percentage |
Gender |
Percentage |
||
Male |
Female |
Male |
Female |
|||
Sport
Bowling
Baseball
Basketball
Track & Field
Softball
Total |
40
26
20
10
15
111 |
36.0
23.4
18.0
9.0
13.5
100.0 |
25
26
11
5
0
67 |
15
0
9
5
15
44 |
22.5
23.0
10.0
4.5
0
60.0 |
13.5
0
8.0
4.5
13.5
40.0 |
Note. Frequency
represents the number of members on each athletic team.
Table 1 also
provides the percentage that each athletic team makes up of the total sample.
The number of male and female members of each team is provided along with the
percentages in which both genders make up the total population of each team and
sample.