Individual Reactions to Adversity: An Exploration of the Relationship
Between Self-Esteem, Resilience, and Locus of Control
Lena M. Faitz
Abstract
The present study hypothesized that a significant relationship exists
between high-self esteem, high resilience, and an internal locus of control. A
total of 98 participants from an undergraduate population at a small, midwestern
university filled out a survey; 51 were men and 47 were women. A Pearson
bivariate correlation was completed and a significant correlation between total
self-esteem and resilience was found. An independent samples t-test was also
conducted to analyze the possibility of significant differences between total
self-esteem, total inner locus of control, and total outer locus of control
scores; a statistically significant difference was detected. Results indicated
that as self-esteem increases, so does resilience; furthermore, high-self esteem
and resilience may be a predictor of an internal locus of control.
Keywords:
self-esteem, resiliency, locus of control, adversity, perception
The purpose
of this study was to determine whether or not there is a relationship among the
concepts of self-esteem, locus of control, and resilience within individuals.
Historically, researchers found that neuroticism was highly correlated with
introversion (Eysenck, 1970). These findings seemed puzzling to this author
because it would seem that introverts tended to exert an inner locus of control
more than their extraverted counterparts, thus indicating that they would be
more emotionally stable, rather than less. By blending the concept of cognitions
and perceptions, the present hypotheses grew into the conjecture that
individuals who exhibit an inner locus of control would be more likely to have
higher self-esteem than those who did not. Additionally, it would seem that
self-esteem and perceived locus of control would be possibly related to traits
of resilience. Each portion of the separate hypotheses will be discussed briefly
in the following sections.
Self-Esteem
Perhaps one
of the most familiar psychological concepts used in modern society is that of
self-esteem (SE); there have been innumerable self-help books published,
speeches delivered, and certainly, research conducted, in an attempt to
understand it (e.g., McKay and Fanning, 2000; Newton, 2011; Baumeister, Smart,
and Boden, 1996). Although there may be little question regarding its validity
as a construct, widespread debate and exploration remains amongst professionals
regarding its influences and overall definition. One conceptualization of
self-esteem is the extent of "people's evaluations of their own self-worth -
that is, the extent to which they view themselves as good, competent, and
decent" (Sciangula & Morry, 2009, p. 144). Individuals with high self-esteem
feel good about themselves and view their lives as worthwhile and feel a sense
of self-respect, despite being aware of their personal flaws. Conversely, those
with low self-esteem allow themselves to be ruled by their perceived weaknesses,
in addition to their feelings of being inadequate, unworthy, and a general
feeling of deficiency (Sciangula & Morry, 2009). In simple terms, self-esteem is
defined as the overall summary of how a person feels about oneself; it can be
conceptualized as a continuum that slightly fluctuates around a general area for
each individual, depending on what sort of situations are encountered.
While most
researchers would agree that people tend to maintain a general self-attitude,
some have examined the possibility of it being a multi-faceted concept. Some
view self-esteem as a two-dimensional construct that consists of a sense of
social worth that is accompanied by the feeling of personal efficacy and power (Ramsdal,
2008). In this study, the author draws an important conclusion relevant to the
present study; self-concept is described as the balance of an individual's ideal
goals and those that have been met. Ideally, one achieves goals, which helps to
build a sense of high competence, which is related to the efficacy portion of
self-esteem (Ramsdal, 2008).
Moreover,
another way that researchers have become more detailed in their definitions of
self-esteem is the development of a distinction between direct and indirect
measures of self-worth. Direct or explicit self-esteem (ESE) is conceptualized
as the traditional measure of how an individual feels about themselves, but it
has been criticized as being inaccurately reported and takes social awareness
too much into consideration (Lebel, 2010). Implicit self-esteem (ISE) has been
touted to be more effective in truly discerning these internal feelings because
it uses introspection to separate actual associations about the self and the
nature of whether they are good are bad. Additionally, individuals seem to
misreport themselves less in ISE measures than in ESE and it seems as though it
may be easier to help with research regarding depression, mental and physical
health, anxiety, feedback sensitivity, or the universality of self-esteem (Lebel,
2010).
Resilience
and Self-Esteem
Although
associated with self-esteem, feedback sensitivity, anxiety, and depression are
all related to the concept of resilience, which is defined as the maintenance of
positive attitude and adaptation despite an individual's experience with
significant adversity or traumatic experience (Luthar, Cicchetti, & Becker,
2000). Stressful situations fully encompass the realms of abuse, neglect, war,
life threats, sexual assault, and death, in addition to covering more mild life
events such as failure, pressure, or striving for achievement. Naturally,
individuals placed within threatening environments may experience anxiety, fear,
or fall into depression. However, some people within these surroundings exhibit
remarkable amounts of resilience and seem to be unscathed by what they have
witnessed or lived. This strength in a person is thought to be produced by
protective factors such as "good intelligence, good communication and
problem-solving skills, the capacity to engage others in relationship...
self-regulation in infancy, interpersonal awareness, the ability to plan,...
determination to be different from abusive parents, and ego 'restriction' or 'overcontrol'
(Eisold, 2003, p. 412)." Each of these tenets have been supported by multiple
research efforts mentioned in Eisold's article (2003) and play a large role,
particularly in children and young adults, in mediating the effects of a
negative environment on the individual.
Although
mental and personality buffers seem to exist and positively correlate with the
resiliency traits, it has been observed that self-esteem (SE) is also related to
the amount of impact that anxiety-producing events and stimuli have on a person
(Richardson, Ratner, & Zumbo, 2009; Brown, 2010; Ramsdal, 2008). In the Brown
study (2010), it was shown that individuals with high self-esteem suffered less
emotional distress than their lower-esteemed counterparts when faced with
negative life events. Such results indicate quite simply that SE is correlated
with the manner in which one may react to a situation; those who do not feel as
secure in themselves would be inclined to feel threatened by negative feedback,
while those who reported higher SE appeared to be less affected overall (Brown,
2010).
Moreover,
the concept of self-competence, which is defined as the way that an individual
feels about his or her goals and accomplishments (Ramsdal, 2008), is yet another
topic within research that bears mentioning. Although seemingly unrelated to
self-esteem, is it actually quite similar, especially with the possibility of
SE's two-dimensional facets of self-efficacy and power. It is asserted that high
levels of perceived competency may be associated with being protected against
potentially stressful situations or as a road to emotional instability and
depression. If an individual assesses his or her goals and comes to the
conclusion that they have achieved relevant goals, feelings of self-competence
increase, which has been associated with a buffering effect against negative
environments. Conversely, if there is a low feeling of self-competence, which
could be extended to overall SE, a person may become more susceptible to anxiety
or depression (Ramsdal, 2008). In another study conducted by Weber, Puskar, and
Ren (2010) about rural adolescents and young adults, the collected data
indicated that there is a strong correlations between depressive symptoms and
low SE, while optimism was related to high SE.
Finally,
another concept that is very closely related to self-esteem is that of
self-compassion (SC), which is defined as the ability for an individual to be
okay with feelings of suffering and regard them with "warmth and concern" (Neff
& McGehee, 2010, p. 226). This particular research has proposed that
self-compassion is more effective than SE in maintaining positive cognitions and
that it was very stable in preserving feelings of self-worth over long periods
of time. More specifically, self-compassion was found to promote resilience
among adolescents, as it offered the ability to cope with social pressures,
feelings of self-consciousness, rumination, anger, and close-mindedness (Neff &
McGehee, 2010). Taking into consideration the effect of self-concept in regards
to negative feedback, it seems to be quite clear that there is a strong
relationship between the two personality constructs.
Locus of
Control and Self-Esteem
Although
self-esteem has been shown to be highly correlated with measures of resilience,
it would be almost negligent to overlook factors which seem to operate in tandem
with SE; locus of control (LOC) is one in particular that seems to have a high
positive correlation with SE. LOC is defined as "the extent to which someone
believes that outcomes are based on his or her own actions or 'personal
characteristics versus the degree to which person expect that the reinforcement
or outcome is a function of chance, luck, or fate, is under control of powerful
others, or is simply unpredictable'" (Wallace, Barry, Zeigler-Hill, & Green,
2012, pg. 213). Individuals who exhibit an internal locus of control have a
tendency to believe that their lives are under their own control and that they
are ultimately the masters of their own destiny. Conversely, those who have an
external LOC may feel that their lives are dictated by randomness and luck,
other individuals who are typically seen as possessing great control and power
over the individual, or that their fates are completely unpredictable (Wallace,
et al., 2012). It has been shown that individuals who have a high SE also tend
to be associated an internal LOC and a negative correlation with depression and
anxiety in adults, while those with low SE have exhibited an external LOC
(Judge, Erez, & Bono, 2002; Wallace, et al., 2012; Simpson, Hillman, Crawford, &
Overton, 2010). Overall, it appears as though a large area of self-esteem lies
within perceived control over a situation and stressful events.
Another
study was conducted by Hames and Joiner (2012) measured the reactions of
individuals with high and low self-esteem to positive self-statements, such as
"I am a lovable person." Participants in the study were exposed to a failure
stimuli and it was reported that those with low SE (the lower 15th percentile)
reacted most negatively toward the criticism. A correlation was discovered
between those with low SE who had been asked to write positive statements about
themselves reported having lower self-esteem after completing the exercise.
However, those with high SE reported increased scores on the Rosenberg
Self-Esteem Scale (RSES) afterwards.
During analysis, it was found that the most effective self-esteem boosting
activity for the individuals with low SE was that of writing about a favorite
past time, instead of specific or non-specific statements. (Hames & Joiner,
2012). These results indicate that people with lower self-esteem tend to respond
more favorably to statements that exhibit the orientation of an external locus
of control. As would be expected, participants with high self-esteem reported
higher measures on the RSES when writing about positive self-statements, which
would be indicative of the presence of an inner LOC; furthermore, they did not
respond as favorably to the other activities (Hames & Joiner, 2012).
Despite the
literature that has explicit and implicit support of the hypothesis that locus
of control and self-esteem are interrelated, there is a presence of dissenting
opinions regarding perceived control and self-efficacy. Ajzen (2002) draws
inspiration from Bandura and other behaviorists for inspiration and influence
upon his concept of perceived behavioral control or theory of planned behavior.
Without delving too deeply into lengthy analysis, the theory describes the three
tenets upon which human behavior is based: behavioral (consequences), normative
(social expectations), and control beliefs (limitations or catalysts of
behavior) (Ajzen, 2002). According to this particular researcher, locus of
control is irrelevant in comparison to behavior; self-efficacy is conceptualized
more as the person's belief that one may successfully behave in a manner that
satisfies all three criteria mentioned above. More specifically, people's
"concern is clearly with control over the behavior itself, not with control over
outcomes or events" (Ajzen, 2002). Although these distinctions are necessary and
helpful to make when determining the definitions of self-efficacy and other
psychological constructs, it could be argued that these finite details do not
particularly matter in the case of self-esteem or resiliency. Locus of control
has been operationalized to function a sort of "blanket term" that could
encompass the attitude that individuals hold about themselves, others, their
fate, and any other concept, tangible or not. Within a smaller lens, Ajzen,
Bandura, and other behaviorists are right to make such a distinction between an
individual's cognitions regarding outcomes and actual behavior. However, in a
more general sense, they are discussing the exact same tenets regarding LOC and
are perhaps expanding the conception of the inner locus.
Locus of
Control and Resilience
As
previously mentioned, external locus of control has been found to have a
positive correlation between negative responses to threatening or stressful
stimuli, such as aggression or depression (Wallace, et al., 2012; Richardson, et
al., 2009). Most of the studies cited in literature have been conducted with
consideration to traumatic life events such as abuse, sexual assault, etc. Few
have examined closely the relationship between resilience personality traits and
locus of control in regards to normal life stress. Leontopoulou (2006) decided
to approach the less damaging side of resilience effects and examined coping
strategies within Greek youth during the transitional period of time (a high
school setting to university and being away from home). The results of the study
indicated strong support for the multiple pieces of literature that assert that
locus of control highly correlates with stress management, coping, and lack of
emotional variability (neuroticism). It was stated that "internal control seemed
to allow adolescents to 'actively deal' with stressful situations so that their
adaptation increased with diversity... [it was suggested] that adaptation was a
strong correlate of locus of inner control at high levels of adversity... [while
at] low levels of internal control...the ability to adapt well was drastically
reduced" (Leontopoulou, 2006). These findings support the hypothesis that the
more control an individual perceives over a situation, he or she will approach
situations in a calm, mentally-healthy manner and will adapt and eventually
overcome. Similar studies (Abouserie, 1994; Diehl & Hay, 2010) showed similar
results that indicated a significant positive correlation between external locus
of control and higher stress levels; those who exhibited internal control
reported less stress than their external counterparts. Additionally, the same
study also demonstrated adequate significance between self-esteem and stress
levels; individuals with higher SE stressed less than those with lower SE (Abouserie,
1994). In cases regarding actual traumatic experiences, being aligned with the
perception of control over the present was associated most greatly with the
ability to adapt to situations (Frazier, Keenan, Anders, Perera, Shallcross, &
Hintz; 2011). The feeling of having control over a current environment would be,
as expected, associated with a strong inner locus of control; focus on the past
or future would be more indicative of external control beliefs.
Present Study
The
expansive amount of literature appears to support the current hypothesis that
measures of an individual's self-esteem and locus of control would be indicative
of his or her capacity for resilience. According to Judge and Bono (2001), an
individual who has high SE is likely to view challenges as an opportunity for
growth; however, those with low SE would view adversity with contempt or feel as
though they will absolutely fail at the task. These attitudes, while related to
resilient characteristics, are also pointing at locus of control. A person who
has high self-esteem will likely hold internal control, thus giving them the
feeling that they are able to successfully complete a task. Due to the
connections demonstrated between SE and LOC in the literature, it is logically
reasonable to expect that because high self-esteem is positively correlated with
high measures of locus control, an person would also exhibit resilient
characteristics; the opposite would also be highly likely (Judge, et al., 2002).
Thus, the present study hypothesizes that there will be a positive correlation
between measures of self-esteem and locus of control in conjunction with
resilience. More specifically, an individual who self-reports high levels of
self-esteem, as measured by Rosenberg, will also report similar scores of
resilience, as measured by Wagnild and Young, and exhibit an internal locus of
control, as measured by Levenson.
Method
Participants
The study
was conducted on a small midwestern college campus; individuals who chose to
participate in the survey were of at least 18 years of age. The only
participant-attribute question that was included in the survey was that of
gender. There was a total of 98 participants; 51 men and 47 women who chose to
fill out the questionnaire were chosen from introductory and required classes
for graduation at the university, in addition to a few upper-level courses,
dining halls, and dorm rooms. Individuals choosing to answer the survey were
part of a convenience sample of undergraduate students. Data were collected in
accordance with the ethical standards of the American Psychological Association
(American Psychological Association, 2010).
Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale.
The Rosenberg Self-Esteem scale has been in use
since 1965 and is one of the most widely-used measures of global self-esteem; it
comes in a variety of item levels, ranging from ten, six, and five. Each item
response is scored with a negative or positive end of the spectrum (Richardson,
et al., 2009). Participants select the amount with which they agree with certain
statements about themselves; some of the items provided are, "I feel that I have
a good number of qualities," and "All in all, I am inclined to feel I am a
failure" (Rosenberg, 1989). Individuals who report a lower number on the scale
are determined to have lower global self-esteem than those who had higher
scores; the range of possible scores is ten through 40. The present version of
the test has been adapted to include a seven-point Likert scale, for consistency
and masking of the study's true purpose.
Levenson Multidimensional Locus of Control Inventory.
The Levenson measure was
developed to measure levels of perceived control; the three modes of perception
are Internal, Powerful Others (external), and Chance (external; Judge, et al.,
2002). Some of the items included are, "Getting what I want requires
pleasing those people above me," and "When I get what I want, it's usually
because I worked hard for it" (Levenson, 1981). Individuals who report high
agreement with the statements reflective of an Internal position are determined
to exhibit an inner locus of control, which chiefly means that they feel that
their lives are determined mainly by their own actions and decisions. The same
principle applies to the other two orientations, which are rather
self-explanatory. The present version of the test has been adapted to include a
seven-point Likert scale so as to render more precise levels of inner or outer
LOC; the lowest score is 23 and the highest is 161, but these numbers are
insignificant to the actual psychometric quality of the test.
Wagnild and Young Resilience Scale. The Wagnild and Young
Resilience Scale was developed to measure an individual's ability to "bounce
back" from stressful or traumatic life experiences. Responses are measured on a
seven-point Likert scale, where lower scores indicate a lesser ability to
recover from negative events and higher scores mean a person is more likely to
recover quickly. Some of the items within the questionnaire are, "I feel proud
that I have accomplished things in life," and "I am friends with myself" (Wagnild
and Young, 1987). The present researcher added a few items to this portion of
the survey in an attempt to mask the true purpose of the study; without the
decoy questions, it is a 14-item measure, where the lowest score is 14 and the
highest is 98.
Procedure
Participants
in this study were asked to fill out a short questionnaire that consisted of
self-report measures of self-esteem, locus of control, and resilience. The only
consistent conditions present were that the primary researcher explained to them
that they must fill out the participation consent forms prior to answering the
survey; she also included a debriefing statement for each session of data
collection. Once all of the surveys have been entered into a statistical
software program (SPSS), individuals will be separated into the following
categories: Low SE (self-esteem), High SE, Inner LOC (locus of control), PO LOC
(Powerful Others), C LOC (Chance), High Resil, and Low Resil. Results of the
study should be easily replicated if the attached survey is distributed to a
convenience sample of undergraduate students.
Results
The present
study hypothesized that individuals reporting high levels of self-esteem, as
measured by Rosenberg, will also report similar scores of resilience, as
measured by Wagnild and Young, and exhibit an internal locus of control, as
measured by Levenson. Each section of the main question was broken down into two
smaller, operationalized hypotheses to analyze better the possible relationships
between each factor. The first, referred to as SE-R, compared total scores of
self-esteem, as measured by Rosenberg, for correlation with total scores of
resilience, as measured by Wagnild and Young. The second, referred to as SE-LOC,
tested the possibility of a relationship between total self-esteem scores, as
measured by Rosenberg, and total inner locus of control scores, as measured by
Levenson. Overall descriptive statistics, according to Table 1, indicated that
the majority of the participants reported mildly high levels of self esteem
(Mean = 52.8, SD = 12.2), mildly high resilience scores (Mean = 81.7, SD =
12.2), and displayed high inner loci of control (Mean = 8.14, SD = 5.5) and a
low external loci of control (Mean = -12.5, SD = 14.97). It is important to note
that the interpretation of LOC scores is unlike most scales due to scores being
both negative and positive. A six-point Likert scale was adapted for simplicity
in administering the surveys, but the responses were re-coded so that 1
(strongly disagree) = -3, 2 (disagree) = -2,... 5 (agree) = +2, and 6 (strongly
agree) = +3. Positive overall scores indicate a stronger tendency toward a
particular locus, while negatives indicate a less strong tendency. Therefore,
most individuals within the survey rejected the statements regarding external
loci of control (powerful others and chance).
Table
1 Descriptive Statistics |
||||||
|
N |
Range |
Minimum |
Maximum |
Mean |
Std. Deviation |
resil
tot |
98 |
54.00 |
50.00 |
104.00 |
81.6837 |
12.17281 |
se
total |
98 |
47.00 |
23.00 |
70.00 |
52.8469 |
12.15056 |
inner
tot |
98 |
30.00 |
-9.00 |
21.00 |
8.1429 |
5.53899 |
external total |
98 |
80.00 |
-44.00 |
36.00 |
-12.4592 |
14.97363 |
Valid
N (listwise) |
98 |
|
|
|
|
|
The first hypothesis, SE-R, posits that high self-esteem scores, as
measured by Rosenberg, will be correlated with high resilience scores, as
measured by Wagnild and Young. A Pearson bivariate correlation was performed
between total self-esteem scores and total resilience scores; this analysis
indicated a strong positive correlation between self-esteem (Mean = 52.84, SD =
12.15) and resilience (Mean = 81.68, SD = 12.17), r(df = 96) = .766, p < .001.
The result, as shown in Table 2, means that as self-esteem increases, so does
resilience; the inverse is also true that if self-esteem decreases, so should
resilience. Therefore, the SE-R hypothesis was supported.
Table 2
Correlations |
|||
|
se total |
resil tot |
|
se
total |
Pearson Correlation |
1 |
.766** |
Sig.
(2-tailed) |
|
.000 |
|
N |
98 |
98 |
|
resil
tot |
Pearson Correlation |
.766** |
1 |
Sig.
(2-tailed) |
.000 |
|
|
N |
98 |
98 |
|
**.
Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed). |
The second
hypothesis, SE-LOC, posited that high self-esteem scores, as measured by
Rosenberg, will be related to the presence of an inner locus of control, as
measured by Levenson. An independent samples t-test analysis comparing total
self-esteem scores and inner and outer locus of control scores, as shown in
Table 3, indicated that inner locus of control scores (Mean = 54.34, SD = 11.18)
and external locus of control scores (Mean = 38.0, SD = 11.89) differed
significantly, based on total self-esteem scores (Mean = 52.84, SD = 12.15),
t(96), 4.157, p < 0.01. These results support the SE-LOC hypothesis that high
self-esteem scores are related to the presence of a high inner locus of control.
The converse may also be true that with decreased self-esteem, there is a
significant relationship with the presence of an external locus of control.
Group Statistics |
|||||
|
LOCoverall |
N |
Mean |
Std. Deviation |
Std. Error Mean |
se total |
inner |
89 |
54.3483 |
11.18248 |
1.18534 |
outer |
9 |
38.0000 |
11.89538 |
3.96513 |
Table 3
Independent Samples Test |
||||||||||
|
Levene's Test for Equality of Variances |
t-test for Equality of Means |
||||||||
F |
Sig. |
t |
df |
Sig. (2-tailed) |
Mean Difference |
Std. Error Difference |
95% Confidence Interval of the Difference |
|||
Lower |
Upper |
|||||||||
se
total |
Equal
variances assumed |
.009 |
.927 |
4.157 |
96 |
.000 |
16.34831 |
3.93281 |
8.54176 |
24.15487 |
Equal
variances not assumed |
|
|
3.950 |
9.487 |
.003 |
16.34831 |
4.13851 |
7.05909 |
25.63754 |
Discussion
The present
study's goal was to analyze the possible significant relationships between the
factors of self-esteem, resilience, and locus of control. Statistical analyses
were conducted and
the main hypothesis that high self-esteem and resilience is associated
with the presence of an inner locus of control was supported. Additionally, a
two-tailed Person's bivariate correlation and an independent samples t-test
yields the conclusion that the findings can be reversed; therefore, low
self-esteem and resilience is associated with an external locus of control.
These findings are consistent with the literature previously discussed in the
beginning on this study and seems as though it has served as properly drawn
connections between each concept.
As with all
studies, this one had several small problems that may have affected the data
collected. The first limitation is that the survey had two formatting errors;
one of the questions was missing the "7" in its Likert scale and another was
split between a page break, thus making it difficult to answer. Several of the
participants did not know what to do when answering these items, so they decided
to not respond with anything. Another problem was that the order of the
questions for each of the pre-established measures was changed; however, it may
be possible that the order of items may have unintentionally lead respondents to
answer differently. Furthermore, some statements, such as "I keep interested in
things," from the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Test, were reversed, which may have made
the survey more confusing than necessary. Overall, most of these errors were
characteristic of a first-time independent research project and are to be
expected.
Despite its
flaws, the present study satisfactorily conducted an original, APA-style
research project and was able to identify significant findings and strong
support for its main hypothesis. One idea that may be helpful in future research
is the incorporation of a different measure for self-esteem; implicit
self-esteem may be a better way to measure cognitions and could indicate other
factors that contribute to its relationship with resilience and locus of
control. Furthermore, the implications of these findings may be extended past
the topics discussed and be applied to areas that study crises of the self,
cognitive behavioral therapy, and a host of other topics. This study has
established a foundation upon which meaningful and highly flexible research can
be conducted; it could be akin to a hallway with nothing but open doors on each
side.
References
Abouserie, R. (1994). Sources and Levels of Stress in Relation to Locus of Control and Self Esteem in University Students. Educational Psychology, 14(3), 323-330.
Ajzen, I. (2002). Perceived Behavioral Control, Self-Efficacy, Locus of
Control, and the Theory of Planned Behavior.
Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32(4),
665-683.
American Psychological
Association (2010). Ethical
Principles for Psychologists and Code of Conduct,
with 2010 Amendments. Retrieved
from www.apa.org/ethics/code2002.html.
Baumeister, R., Smart, L., and Boden, J. (1996). Relation of threatened egotism to violence and aggression: The dark side of high self-esteem. Psychological Review, 103(1), 5-33.
Brown, J. (2010). High self-esteem buffers negative feedback: Once more with feeling. Cognition and Emotion, 24(8), 1389-1404.
Diehl, M. and Hay, E. (2010). Risk and Resilience Factors in Coping With Daily Stress in Adulthood: The Role of Age, Self-Concept Incoherence, and Personal Control. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 1132-1146.
Eisold, B. (2005). NOTES ON LIFELONG RESILIENCE. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 22(3), 411-425.
Frazier, P., Keenan, N., Anders, S., Perera, S., Shallcross, S., and Hintz, S. (2011). Perceived Past, Present, and Future Control and Adjustment to Stressful Life Events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(4), 749-765.
Hames, J. and Joiner, T. (2012). Resilience factors may differ as a
function of self-esteem level: testing the efficacy of two types of positive
self-statements following a laboratory stressor.
Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 31(6), 641-662.
Judge, T., Erez, A., Bono, J., and Thoresen, C. (2002). Are Measures of
Self-Esteem, Neuroticism, Locus of Control, and Generalized Self-Efficacy
Indicators of a Common Core Construct? Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 83(3), 693-710.
Lebel, E. (2010). Attitude Accessibility as a Moderator of Implicit and Explicit Self-esteem Correspondence. Self and Identity, 9(1), 195-208.
Leontopoulou, S. (2006). RESILIENCE OF GREEK YOUTH AT AN EDUCATIONAL TRANSITION POINT: THE ROLE OF LOCUS OF CONTROL AND COPING STRATEGIES AS RESOURCES. Social Indicators Research, 76(1), 95-126. doi:
Luthar, S., Cicchetti, D., and Becker, B. (2000). The Construct of
Resilience: A Critical Evaluation and Guidelines for Future Work. Child
Development, 71(3), 543-562.
McKay, M. & Fanning, P. (2000).
Self-Esteem: A Proven Program of Cognitive Techniques for
Assessing, Improving,
and Maintaining Your Self-Esteem.
(3rd ed). New Harbinger Publications.
Neff, K. and McGehee, P. (2010). Self-compassion and Psychological Resilience Among Adolescents and Young Adults. Self and Identity, 9(1), 225-240.
Newton, T. (Performer) (2011, July). Embracing otherness, embracing myself. TED
Talks. [Video podcast]. Retrieved
from
http://www.ted.com/talks/thandie_newton_embracing_otherness_embracing_myself.html
Ramsdal, G. (2008). Differential relations between two dimensions of self-esteem and the Big Five? Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 49(1), 333-338.
Richardson, C., Ratner, P., and Zumbo, B. (2009). Further Support for Multidimensionality Within the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale. Current Psychology, 28(1), 98-114.
Sciangula, A. and Morry, Marian. (2009). Self-Esteem and Perceived Regard: How I
See Myself Affects My Relationship Satisfaction. The Journal of Social
Psychology, 149(2), 143-158.
Simpson, J., Hillman, R., Crawford, T., and Overton, P.G. (2010). Self-esteem and self-disgust both mediate the relationship between dysfunctional cognitions and depressive symptoms. Motivation and Emotion, 34(1), 399-406.
Wallace, M., Christopher, B., Zeigler-Hill, V., and Green, B. (2012). Locus of Control as a Contributing Factor in the Relation Between Self-Perception and Adolescent Aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 38(1), 213-221.
Weber, S., Puskar, K., and Ren, D. (2010). Relationships Between Depressive Symptoms and Perceived Social Support, Self-Esteem, and Optimism in a Sample of Rural Adolescents. Issues in Mental Health Nursing, 31(1), 584-588.